Background

Psychopath’s “Karma” Confirmed in Study (Patrick’s Triarchic Model, Hyperarousal)

Uploaded 4/13/2024, approx. 29 minute read

The myth that psychopaths are fearless has been debunked for almost a decade in a slew of studies. And today we understand that most psychopaths actually experience anxiety. Some of them even have anxiety disorder.

Indeed, my personal point of view is that psychopathy is a condition of hyperarousal, overreactivity, hyperreactivity to anxiety and to fear. The psychopath's brain disables the centers of the brain that deal with fear and anxiety because there is a threat that the psychopath would be overwhelmed.

The linkage between anxiety and psychopathy is well established. Not so, the linkage between fear or the absence of fear and psychopathy, a topic that is currently a hot button issue in psychology.

I believe ultimately we will find out that psychopathy is a mechanism, a coping strategy or a coping mechanism that allows the psychopath to survive in high anxiety, fearful, terrifying environments by disabling parts of his or her brain. Mostly his, most psychopaths are still men.

As a medical condition, it's known as Urbach-Wiethe, named after the two doctors who have discovered it. And in this disease, there's an accumulation of fatty residues, cells malfunction and a lot of fat accumulates throughout the body under the skin, etc. And finally, reaches the brain, invades the brain. This fatty tissue accumulates in the brain and destroys the amygdala, center of the brain that is in charge, among other things, of fear and arousal and emotions and other issues.

So patients with Urbach-Wiethe condition syndrome or disease are incapable of experiencing fear except the most basic forms of fear, like fear of drowning. Or when in clinical experiments they are asked to breathe in carbon dioxide instead of oxygen, then they display fear. It's a kind of reptilian fear, if you wish, but otherwise they're fearless.

And so many, many scholars and doctors and psychiatrists and so on have been comparing Urbach-Wiethe patients with psychopaths, which is a serious mistake.

It is true that the psychopath's amygdala, amygdalae, multiple, are actually less active than the normal person.

But it begs the question, what precedes what? Does the dysfunction of the amygdalae precede, or precedes, does it precede the psychopathy? Or is psychopathy causing the brain's structural and functional abnormalities?

That's the $64,000 question. And this is the core of the philosophy of neuroscience.

We observe the brain, we analyze it, we record a variety of functional parameters, multi-unit activity and blood flows in fMRI and so on and so forth. But we have no way of telling what is the cause and what is the effect.

Is the mental health condition the cause of the changes in the brain or are the changes in the brain the causes of the mental health condition?

And this applies to psychopaths as well.


And today I want to share with you a recent study published in the academic magazine, academic journal, Personality and Individual Differences. It was published last month in March 2024.

It's titled The Personal Cost of Psychopathy: Impacts of Triarchic Psychopathy Traits on Impairment, Internalizing, and Life Satisfaction.

The authors are Kelsey Davis, Nicholas Kavish, and Jaime Anderson.

So it's a very interesting question because we all focus on the cost of psychopathy to society, on the cost paid by victims of psychopaths.

But we never ask the question, what does it feel like to be a psychopath? What is the personal cost that the psychopath pays for his or more rarely her psychopathy?

And this, as I said, is the topic of this study.

So the new study is fascinating to read. It discusses the personal repercussions of living with psychopathic traits.

Now a caveat, the study has been conducted on undergraduate students, which is a unique cohort and many professors would tell you a bit weird. But anyhow, undergraduate students, and so it is not representative of humanity at large. It's largely self-selecting sample. These are people who have chosen to study and then have chosen to participate in the research. And it's highly limited in terms of age, location, more Western, and so on and so forth.

So bear this in mind as we proceed.

But the study tried to correlate psychopathic personality traits and individuals functioning across various domains. It tried to somehow discern or tweak out the impact of psychopathy on interpersonal relationships, internalizing behaviors, overall quality of life.

Now psychopathy is a constellation of traits and behaviors. Psychopathy is a behavioral relational social disorder, which brings many scholars to the realization that it's actually not a mental illness, not a clinical entity. It's more of a social dysfunction or actually perhaps a lifestyle or a social choice.

The psychopath opts out of society. The psychopath chooses to defy authority, is contumacious, is defiant, is reckless, and so on and so forth. These are character traits to a large extent. They even include a temperamental component, but is it a mental illness? It's highly debatable.

Superficial charm, lack of empathy and remorse, manipulativeness, Machiavellianism, egocentricity, impulsivity, antisocial behaviors. They're not nice. None of this is nice, but is any of this mental illness? I beg to differ. I don't think so.

Psychopathy has been studied. It's actually the first mental health condition to have ever been studied. We have well over 200 studies of psychopathy, as I've repeatedly reminded you in previous videos over the years.

But the main focus was the impact on society, the link between psychopathic traits, psychopathic behaviors such as criminal behaviors, violent behaviors, aggressive behaviors, and the unfortunate outcomes, the price that victims and targets and marks pay.

But now there's a growing recognition that we need to understand the inner world of the psychopath the same way some of us, myself included, have been trying to understand the inner world and the inner dynamics of the narcissist.

How do these traits affect individuals who possess them? Do these individual possess the traits or do these traits possess the individuals?

The authors of the study say most psychopathic traits are generally undesirable, although this has primarily been asserted based on the consequences to society. The individual consequences may prove to be just as impactful.

So the study based itself on what is known as a triarchic model of psychopathy. And this model breaks down psychopathy into three core dimensions, boldness, daring, disinhibition, and meanness being nasty like me.

So boldness has to do, or is closely affiliated or related to fearlessness, social dominance, emotional resilience in the face of adversity.

When you're high on boldness, you have a strong sense of self-confidence, almost a belief, a supernatural belief that you're immune to the consequences of your actions. Boldness is a bit stupid.

And there's a tolerance for the unfamiliar, for the risky, for the dangerous situations, events, people, and there's a capacity to remain calm, collected, and assertive under pressure, even unusual pressure. This is boldness.

Disinhibition is almost another word, almost a synonym of impulsivity, lack of restraint, poor behavioral control. Freud would have said that this is a manifestation of the id when the ego is not functioning.

And this dimension of disinhibition reflects an inability, an innate inability, not a lack of will, but an inability, structural, constitutional, built-in, hard-baked, hard-wired, baked-in inability to resist immediate, short-term temptations and opportunities.

So it's opportunism that is risky and dangerous.

Individuals that have high disinhibition struggle with planning, with foresight, and they act without considering the consequences of their actions.

It brings to mind an alcoholic.

Indeed, in alcoholism, we have a phenomenon known as alcohol myopia. When you act, the executive functions are out of control of higher functions.

And so you act as if you were immune, as if you are untouchable, as if you're godlike.

It's very common among alcoholics or people who have been drinking.


And finally, the last dimension.

You remember a triarchic model, three dimensions, boldness, disinhibition.

And the last dimension is meanness or nastiness or some wackniness.

It's a lack of empathy, disdain for close relationships, contempt of other people who are regarded as inferior and unworthy of consideration and compassion, readiness to use and exploit others for personal gain, callousness, ruthlessness, goal orientation, which excludes the cause of other people as a relevant factor or a relevant consideration.

And this dimension captures the uncaring and exploitative aspects of psychopathy, their callousness.

Individuals with high level of meanness are indifferent to the harm that they cause to others.

They often engage in manipulative, deceitful, predatory behavior. They're often also aggressive or at the very least passive aggressive.

The authors say many previous studies have been limited by only reporting a total score of psychopathy or by utilizing a two-factor model.

They're referring to Robert Hare's work.

This study expands current knowledge by using the dimensional, triarchic model by examining a person's specific variations in traits of meanness, boldness and disinhibition.

Insight can be gained into the potential impairments they may face.

And of these three, it seems that the most problematic is disinhibition.

Disinhibition is detrimental to the psychopath.

It is strongly and significantly associated with a range of negative outcomes across both studies conducted under the umbrella of this article.

And so when the psychopath is disinhibited, he pays a heavy price.

His impulsive, reckless behavior, which lacks foresight and fore-planning and premeditation sometimes leads him to pay an enormous price in terms of freedom, in terms of property, in terms of reputation, in terms of advancement, in terms of accomplishment, one way or another.

Impulsive, careless, reckless, cruel, callous, ruthless action leads the psychopath to diminishment, self-defeat and self-destruction.

Self-harm, it's a form of self-harming and it raises a very interesting question about the connection between psychopathy and borderline.

People with borderline personality disorder often self-harm.

And when they self-harm, it's usually in the stage known as acting out where the borderline essentially becomes a secondary psychopath.

So here's a linkage between psychopathy and borderline.

It seems that impulsivity is the bridge that connects psychopathy and borderline.

By acting impulsively without foresight, as if you're immune and untouchable, as if there's no tomorrow, you end up paying an extremely heavy price one way or another.

An individual exhibiting higher levels of disinhibition experiences therefore greater impairments in social and personality functioning.

The reduced quality of life across multiple domains, physical, psychological, social, environmental, and they reported higher perceptions of impairment in cognition and life activities such as work or school.

So disinhibition plays a crucial role in the personal costs associated with psychopathy and it affects the psychopath's ability to function effectively in various aspects of life.

In short, disinhibition is the opposite of self-efficacy.

It diminishes, reduces the psychopath's self-efficacy, the psychopath's ability to extract long-term beneficial outcomes from others and from his environment or her environment.

Boldness is a more nuanced trait, a more nuanced dimension of psychopathy.

There's more variability here.

Boldness is linked to negative outcomes of course.

If you're daring, if you're stupidly daring, then it borders, it's almost impulsive, wouldn't you say? Daring and impulsivity are like first cousins or even siblings.

But boldness also is linked to some positive outcomes.

For example, accomplishing things.

So boldness is connected or correlated with a higher quality of life, a lower likelihood of self-harm and therefore could lead an individual to become more self-efficacious.

Unfortunately in psychopathy, boldness is the outcome or highly correlated with challenged empathy and inability to be intimate, problems with intimacy.

So the psychopath's boldness is because he cannot empathize, he cannot be intimate with anyone, he cannot resonate with another person.

And so the psychopath sees no common denominator between himself and anyone else.

It's the same way normal healthy human beings view insects.

The psychopath regards you as some kind of animal, a pet in the best case, an insect in the worst case.

He either has to squash you, trample on you as he would an insect or he has to control you and manipulate you as he would a pet, tame you, domesticate you.

So these problems with empathy and intimacy cause the psychopath to be daring, bold, in your face, not only defiant but defiant in a goal oriented manner and sometimes it brings positive outcomes.

For example, the prevalence and incidence of psychopathy among the chief executive offices of Fortune 500 companies is five times higher than in the general population.

Same applies to medical surgeons. Medical surgeons, there are many psychopaths among them, much more common than in the general population.

And so this duality of boldness that sometimes it's a good thing and sometimes it's a bad thing.

It means that boldness confers certain advantages.

It's a kind of positive adaptation or at the very least not an entirely negative adaptation.

It enhances resilience. It is the cause of fearlessness, this daring, but it also impairs interpersonal relationships and prevents, inhibits emotional connections.

Disinhibition is therefore a kind of inhibition, inhibition when it comes to empathy, inhibition when it comes to intimacy, inhibition when it comes to interpersonal relationships, inhibition when it comes to signaling vulnerability.

So we have disinhibition which is highly correlated with behavioral aspects of psychopathy and we have inhibition in the form of boldness which is highly correlated with the emotional landscape of the psychopath including first and foremost his inability to empathize.

And finally, meanness, nastiness. This is associated, not surprisingly, with a lot of interpersonal difficulties.

This dimension of psychopathy contributes significantly to the psychopath's inability to function socially and problems in social interactions.

But the relationship between meanness or nastiness and other aspects of personal well-being such as quality of life is not that straightforward.

Sometimes being mean and nasty pays. It's a winning strategy.

So there's variability when it comes to the connection between meanness and nastiness and for example accomplishments and accomplishments that lead to enhanced quality of life.

In short, boldness and meanness sometimes work. Sometimes they are actually positive adaptations, the correct coping strategies especially in environments that are hostile, unpredictable, dangerous, risky, unstable, indeterminate such as our entire civilization right now.

None of this applies to disinhibition. Disinhibition is simply stupid. It's self-destructive. It's self-harming.

When you act impulsively without thought and foresight, without analyzing what you're doing, then you're putting yourself in enormous danger and you're likely, you're liable, you're bound to pay a price for this.

And so this is inconsistency between one or two traits which are sometimes good, sometimes useful, sometimes positively adaptive and a trait that is always negative.

This inconsistency highlights the challenge in trying to understand psychopathy.

Meanness and boldness, the effects of meanness and boldness are contingent on context, on culture, on society, on circumstances, on the environment, on other people.

Disinhibition is always bad.

But when we put the three together, we get a much more nuanced mosaic, nuanced kaleidoscopic picture of psychopathy which is not entirely black and white.

It makes it very difficult to split the psychopath, to say psychopaths are all bad, we are all good.

The authors say it appears that impulsive, antisocial and disinhibited traits are most strongly associated with personal costs.

Indeed, they have been tied to more significant impairment, reduced life satisfaction, increased internalizing and high risk of self-harm and suicidality.

Conversely, bold and fearless dominant traits appear potentially protective or more often unrelated to personal costs.


Okay, I would like now to read to you excerpts from the study because it is also an excellent introduction to psychopathy in general.

In the description you will find a link to the article in the literature part.

So the article opens with highlights.

Highness inhibition was the most impairing triarchic psychopathy trait across functioning domains for undergraduates, I remind you.

Number two, the personal effect of increased boldness varied eliciting primarily positive but some negative outcomes.

And meanness nastiness was primarily related to interpersonal difficulties.

The authors say although psychopathic traits have long been acknowledged as a public health concern, a growing body of evidence suggests there is a significant personal cost to living with psychopathy.

However, much of the current literature on psychopathy impairment appears mixed in findings varying by the traits examined and measured outcomes.

The current paper aims to contribute to this field of research by examining how a triarchic model of psychopathy relates to a wide variety of potential consequences including impairment, personality functioning, interpersonal functioning, internalizing and perceived quality of life.

Through two studies utilizing the triarchic psychopathy model in undergraduate sample, significant psychopathy impairment was identified.

Broadly disinhibition was related to the most significant impairments across functioning domains.

Boldness was more variable, elicited both positive, higher quality of life, lower self-harm and negative impairments in empathy and intimacy outcomes.

Meanness was primarily related to interpersonal difficulties.

However differences in measuring meanness led to different findings on its connection to personality functioning.

By better understanding the consequences of psychopathy and how they vary by dimensional traits, interventions can be better tailored to alleviate both individual and societal suffering.


And now I'll read to you the introduction which is as I said an excellent entry into the field of psychopathy.

Psychopathic traits, say the authors, are increasingly recognized as a public health concern due to their connection with externalizing and antisocial behavior.

Now each of these statements is followed by a bibliographic note.

So I recommend that you read the article itself if you want to be exposed to many other points of view.

So they say there's a connection of psychopathy to externalizing and antisocial behavior, largely focusing on the impact of psychopathy at a societal rather than personal level.

However there's a growing evidence that some of these traits may pose a personal cost as well through impaired functioning, internalizing and interpersonal difficulty.

Despite the emergence of concerns, this realm of psychopathy related personal consequences is still emerging and additional evidence is needed to understand the nature of these traits and how they impact the individual.

Part of the difficulty in understanding the personal cost of psychopathy lies in the ambiguity of psychopathy as a construct, say the authors.

Although commonly associated with maladaptive personality traits and disorders, it has also been conceptualized as a constellation of traits rooted in the dimensions of normative personality.

Additionally, other researchers have cited evidence of psychopathy being a developmental disorder, which can be predicted by significant genetic risk factors.

Most modern researchers acknowledge that psychopathy is best understood as a multidimensional construct featuring correlated traits.

However, which specific characteristics create the overarching construct is still debated.

Generally researchers agree there are typically problems in interpersonal, manipulativeness, superficial charm, affective, callousness, lack of remorse and behavioral impulsivity irresponsibility domains.

This has been first identified by Cleckley actually in 1941 followed by Hare in 1996 and later, Patrick and so on.

Regardless of the specific definition of psychopathy used, it remains evident that most psychopathic traits are generally undesirable.

Although this has primarily been asserted based on the consequences to society, the individual consequences may prove to be just as impactful.

Intuitively, the most direct path to conceptualizing the personal cost of psychopathy would lie in impairment, some kind of impairment.

However, say the authors, the pool of research assessing the relation between psychopathy and impairment is limited and mixed in findings.

Broadly speaking, functional impairment has been connected to psychopathy.

Miller and allies examined the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition, newly introduced psychopathy specifier as part of the Section 3 diagnostic model of antisocial personality disorder.

Consistent with priorfearlessdominance-baldness research, the psychopathy specifier was negatively correlated to both self and informant related functional impairment scores.

However, the components of the specifier demonstrated disparate findings.

Control of impairment moderately increased as withdrawal and anxiousness increased. Attention seeking was only weakly positively correlated with impairment.

Now, say the authors, regarding specific facets of impairment, it would seem inherent that psychopathy is associated with empathy impairment.

Psychopaths have been classically characterized as lacking empathy and remorse, often having attenuated responses to emotional stimuli.

However, existing research across various fields such as neurobiology and social psychology has been characterized by mixed findings.

Indeed, within offender populations, Mayer found high psychopathy scores were related to lower self-reported empathy, less altruistic behavior and an attenuated effective response to an empathy induction task.

But in contrast, Domus and others found psychopathy was not associated with either cognitive or emotional empathy in offenders.

Indeed, Borg and Sinnot Armstrong assert current literature does not provide consistent evidence of psychopathy being associated with severe moral cognitive deficits.

Research on intimacy impairment within psychopathy has yielded somewhat more consistent findings.

One of the most intensive examinations of psychopathy's impact on intimacy found those with high psychopathic traits tended to marry one another and psychopathic traits predicted lower marital satisfaction and divorce.

Similarly, in an undergraduate sample, psychopathy accounted for significant in-relationship factors, including overall relationship quality, commitment, satisfaction, romance and trust for men and women.

However, the relation between psychopathy and intimacy impairment can vary by which psychopathy traits are examined.

Ali and Chamorro Primusic found primary psychopathy characterized by traits such as lack of remorse, callousness and manipulative behavior. Primary psychopathy was positively associated with relationship factors such as intimacy, commitment and passion.

However, secondary psychopathy characterized by impulsivity, aggression and hostility negatively predicted intimacy.

Overall it appears that psychopathy can have an impairing impact on an individual far from the previous conceptualization that the traits may be purely beneficial or benign at best.

However, the current findings, such as the differences seen between intimacy, empathy and functional impairment, remain mixed and require further investigation.

The authors say it is generally agreed that internalizing symptoms such as depression, anxiety and withdrawal can have aversive and sometimes debilitating effects on those managing them.

The connection between psychopathic traits and internalizing provides insight into additional personal costs.

However, lack of impairment, this relationship varies depending on the psychopathy trait in question.

Indeed Lutzmann and others used a triarchic approach to psychopathy and they found that disinhibited traits were generally positively related to internalizing.

Meanwhile, boldness traits were negatively associated with broad internalizing and specific internalizing subdomains such as dysphoria, suicidality, panic, general depression.

In juvenile offenders, internalizing and specifically trait anxiety was found to be positively related to higher levels of psychopathy and subsequent recidivism.

Furthermore, Selbaum found evidence that certain psychopathic traits, for example, fearless dominance and so on and so forth, were negatively correlated with both negative affectivity and post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms in a university sample.

PTSD in particular shows a complicated relationship with psychopathy.

Blair and Mitchell suggested that from a theoretical perspective, psychopathy should act as a protective factor from PTSD because patients with PTSD show heightened interference by emotional distractors as opposed to the reduced interference seen in psychopathy.

Some empirical findings support this theory as hospitalized men who scored higher on the psychopathy checklist showed a lower level of traumatic symptomatology despite having experienced a higher number of traumatic events.

By the wayI have a video here on the psychopathic self-state as a protector state and I encourage you to watch it.

The authors continue, "However, when examined from a dimensional perspective, the relation between PTSD and psychopathy is less clear.

Selbaum found impulsive antisociality was positively correlated with PTSD symptoms and fearless dominance was negatively correlated.

Additionally, negative affectivity accounted for the majority of the shared variance for both sub-facets.

However, although Cavish found a positive association between antisocial psychopathic traits and PTSD symptoms severity, they found no evidence of an association with the affective traits of psychopathy.

Depression has also been examined as a correlate of psychopathic traits.

In an undergraduate sample, psychopathy was associated with higher levels of depression and negative affect and lower levels of happiness and positive affect, nothing unexpected.

Cavish and his partners also found a positive relation between antisocial psychopathic traits and depression symptom severity.

However, some researchers have asserted that individuals with higher levels of psychopathic traits are devoid of depressive symptoms.

Indeed, Wiersman found psychopathy checklist revised scores, in particular interpersonal, affective and lifestyle facets, are inversely associated with depression scores in adult male prisoners.

Like other forms of internalizing, suicidality was once thought to be the antithesis of psychopathy, incompatible with psychopathy, suicide, but empirical evidence has suggested otherwise.

Cleckley in 1941 and in 1976 described a defining trait of psychopathy to be suicide rarely carried out, in other words, suicidal ideation.

In further suggested, psychopathic individuals are immune to suicidality.

However, several studies have reported a disparate relationship between psychopathy and suicidality.

Often suicidality is positively related to impulsive antisocial traits, factor two, but negatively related or unrelated to affective interpersonal deficits, factor one.

This relationship has also been examined through the interpersonal psychological theory of suicidal behavior.

Overall, the existing evidence suggests that behavioral traits associated with psychopathy are particularly positively related to risk for suicide, while affective deficits are less or unrelated to suicide.

There is also less evidence relative to the literature on other internalizing symptoms that boldness, fearless dominance traits are protective against suicidality.

The authors conclude, although notably less studied than internalizing symptoms, there is initial evidence of decreased life satisfaction in those with high psychopathic traits.

In online and undergraduate samples, psychopathy accounted for a substantial portion of the overall variance in life satisfaction, with higher psychopathy predicting lower life satisfaction.

However, in a sample of male offenders with psychiatric diagnoses, there were no significant differences in life satisfaction across the severity of psychopathic traits.

Finally, Ali and Chamorro Pramuzic found primary psychopathy was not significantly associated with life satisfaction, although the correlation was in the negative direction.

However, secondary psychopathy negatively predicted life satisfaction.

And this sits well with the hypothesis that the borderline is a secondary psychopathic sub-state, which predicts lower life satisfaction.


Okay, back to the authors.

A growing body of evidence suggests psychopathic traits are differentially related to an array of adverse outcomes.

Recent literature appears to demonstrate that impulsive and antisocial traits are generally associated with lower levels of functioning, greater impairment, reduced life satisfaction, and increased life-harming behaviors.

Conversely, bold and fearless dominant traits seem protective against the same sequelae.

Nevertheless, more research is warranted to further explore the replicability of these findings, etc.

So, this is an excellent introduction to the current state of knowledge of psychopathy and runs contra to many of the nonsensical myths propagated online, in classrooms, in universities, and even in textbooks.

Psychopathy is not what we thought it is. It is closely associated with internalizing effects such as depression and anxiety.

It is somewhat correlated with suicidality.

Life satisfaction is low in some types of psychopathy.

Some elements of psychopathy are actually positive adaptations, others are negative adaptations.

It is a much more nuanced picture and we need to realize that human beings cannot be captured with a single label, cannot be all bad, all good.

That is a splitting defense, even in the profession of psychology, in clinical psychology, psychiatry. We tend to split.

Psychopaths are all bad. Narcissists are all bad.

We demonize, even in the profession. We need to stop doing this.

Human beings are multifaceted, even psychopaths, even narcissists.

Human beings are layers upon layers upon layers of complexities upon complexities.

Human beings are an archaeology site.

Human beings are much more complex than the entire universe.

The brain is the most complex object in the universe that we have ever come across or even conceive of.

We cannot reduce this to nine criteria or to a few sentences and paragraphs.

We cannot perceive human beings as one-dimensional caricatures or cardboard cutouts.

We need to find out, we need to begin to perceive nuance, we need to study a lot more, we need to be humble in the face of mental health and even more so in the face of mental illness.

Thank you.

If you enjoyed this article, you might like the following:

NEW Psychopath: Ashamed, Empathic, Anxious (Primary Factor 1 F1 vs. Secondary F2 Literature Review)

There is a significant shift in the understanding of psychopathy, particularly distinguishing between primary and secondary psychopaths, which may represent two distinct categories rather than variations of the same disorder. Primary psychopaths are characterized by low anxiety and a lack of emotional response to others, while secondary psychopaths experience anxiety and can exhibit emotional responses, including empathy. Recent studies suggest that psychopathy may not solely be an empathy deficit, as some psychopaths can experience emotional empathy, challenging traditional views on the disorder. This evolving understanding emphasizes the need for a nuanced approach to psychopathy, recognizing its complexity and the implications for treatment and societal protection.


Antisocial Psychopath and Sociopath: Antisocial Personality Disorder

Psychopathy is characterized by antisocial traits combined with a lack of empathy, callousness, and deceitfulness, and it often becomes evident in early adolescence. While it is chronic, psychopathy can improve with age, as criminal behavior and substance abuse typically decline in older individuals. Psychopaths view others as tools for their gratification, reject social norms, and lack a sense of obligation or remorse, often rationalizing their harmful actions. Additionally, psychopathy may have genetic and neurological components, and some individuals may exhibit underlying anxiety that drives their antisocial behavior.


Myth of Fearless Psychopath

Psychopaths exhibit forms of dysregulation that can be compared to those seen in borderline personality disorder, but their dysregulation is externalized through impulsive and reckless behaviors, impacting themselves and others negatively. Contrary to the stereotype of fearlessness, psychopaths do experience anxiety and fear, but their physiological responses differ from neurotypical individuals, leading to misinterpretations of their emotional states. They often mislabel their feelings and experiences, confusing emotions like fear with excitement, and perceive threats differently, being hypervigilant towards intimate relationships while taking reckless risks with strangers. Ultimately, psychopaths live in a state of internal chaos, driven by primitive urges and a lack of moral scruples, leading to a profound emptiness and self-destructive tendencies.


High-functioning Autism: Psychopathy? Narcissism?

High-functioning autism (HFA) is often misdiagnosed as narcissistic personality disorder or psychopathy due to similarities in behavior, such as a lack of empathy, brain abnormalities, and criminal behavior. However, there are key differences between HFA and these personality disorders, such as language skills and social functioning. While HFA is a brain disorder with no intellectual disability, narcissism and psychopathy are personality disorders that can be linked to early childhood experiences and trauma. It is important not to make snap judgments when observing someone's behavior, as the distinctions between these disorders are complex and nuanced.


Is Your Pet Cat a Psychopath? (PURR, GASP!)

A recent study by researchers at the University of Liverpool has found that all cats have some level of psychopathy, with the reason being that in an ancestral environment that demanded self-sufficiency, wild cats that had higher levels of psychopathic traits may have been more successful in acquiring resources such as food, territory and mating opportunities. The study settled on the conclusion that all cats are psychopathic to some degree. The questions asked in the study are pretty generic, such as my cat demands attention, or my cat disobeys rules, or behaviors that are pretty standard for the species.


Over-sexed: Histrionic Personality Disorder and Narcissism

Histrionic personality disorder is more commonly diagnosed in women, leading to questions about whether it is a real mental health problem or a reflection of a patriarchal society. Histrionics crave attention and are uncomfortable when not at the center of it, similar to narcissists. They are preoccupied with physical appearance and sexual conquests, and often act flirtatious and seductive. Histrionics are enthusiastic and emotional, but their behavior can be exhausting and off-putting to others.


Narcissist: Is He or Isn't He?

Narcissism is a spectrum of behaviors, from healthy to pathological, and the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual specifies nine diagnostic criteria for narcissistic personality disorder (NPD). A malignant narcissist is someone who has NPD and wreaks havoc on themselves and their surroundings. They feel grandiose and self-important, exaggerate accomplishments, and demand recognition as superior without commensurate achievements. They require excessive admiration, adulation, attention, and affirmation, and are interpersonally exploitative, devoid of empathy, and constantly envious of others.


Dark Pentagram Personality: Dark Triad/Tetrad and Beyond

Professor Sam Vaknin suggests a new construct called the dark pentagram personality, which includes Machiavellianism, subclinical narcissism, subclinical psychopathy, covert narcissism, and borderline personality. He argues that the dark triad and dark tetrad are redundant, as their components are all facets and manifestations of psychopathy. Vaknin believes that sadism should not be included in the dark personality constructs, as it is too similar to psychopathy. Instead, he proposes adding subclinical covert narcissism and subclinical borderline personality to create the dark pentagram personality.


Autism Spectrum Disorder: REAL? (Literature Review)

Pathological narcissism and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are both fields plagued by misinformation and self-proclaimed experts. Recent discoveries indicate that ASD is a neurodevelopmental disorder with complex etiology, including potential correlations with parental age and environmental factors, but no definitive causes have been established. Diagnosis remains challenging due to overlapping symptoms with other conditions and reliance on clinician experience, leading to a high likelihood of misdiagnosis. Current research is exploring genetic, metabolic, and gastrointestinal factors, but the understanding of ASD remains limited and fraught with speculation.


Borderline Mislabels Her Emotions (as do Narcissist, Psychopath)

Empathy is inversely related to the ability to recognize emotions in others, meaning that as empathy increases, the capacity to accurately read others' emotions decreases. Individuals with cluster B personality disorders, such as narcissists and borderlines, possess distorted forms of empathy that hinder their emotional understanding and labeling, leading to significant cognitive and emotional deficits. These individuals often mislabel their emotions, rely on dysfunctional coping mechanisms, and experience emotional dysregulation, resulting in inappropriate affect and a lack of genuine emotional connection. Ultimately, their emotional experiences are characterized by a cognitive analysis rather than true emotive engagement, leaving them disconnected from the richness of human emotional experience.

Transcripts Copyright © Sam Vaknin 2010-2024, under license to William DeGraaf
Website Copyright © William DeGraaf 2022-2024
Get it on Google Play
Privacy policy